Finding the Way Back
Photograph by Wilson Lee / Son Gallery / Getty
The
Harvard professor John Huth first offered his course “Science of the
Physical Universe 26: Primitive Navigation” in 2007. Since then, he has
taught around five hundred undergraduates about the rudiments of
analogue way-finding (sun, stars, tides, weather, wind) in a range of
cultures (Berber, Norse, Polynesian, early European). Huth is an
experimental particle physicist; he was involved in the discovery of
both the top quark and the Higgs boson.
He is also an avid outdoorsman and, when it comes to navigation, a
smartphone and G.P.S. skeptic. “All empiricism has to start with stuff
that is immediately palpable to you,” he told me recently. “The march of
education, especially in the sciences, has been divorced from that
reality, and I think that’s where you have to start.” He began one of
his lectures this spring with a question: “Which way is the wind blowing
outside? Anyone notice?” The assembled students, about fifty in all,
were silent. “Southeast?” one ventured. “Northeast,” Huth said.
As
a species, humans lack many of the biological gifts that allow other
animals to get around. A loggerhead turtle, for example, begins to take
its bearings within a couple of hours of hatching, using magnetite
crystals in its brain to sense Earth’s magnetic field. (Spiny lobsters,
monarch butterflies, and termites have similar compasses.) Honeybees get
from nectar to hive and back in part by judging the position of the
sun, which they can sense, even on a cloudy day, from patterns in
polarized light. Where biology has failed humans, we have substituted
culture. Throughout our evolutionary history, we have created ad-hoc
systems of knowledge that organize environmental information and make it
transmissible to the next generation. Often, difficult and monotonous
landscapes—desert, sea, ice—resulted in more intricate systems. Several
thousand years before the magnetic compass was invented, Pacific
Islanders had worked out how to navigate by star compasses and read
ocean swells for information about nearby land. (Part of Huth’s summer
vacation this year will be spent in the Marshall Islands, learning
similar techniques from local sailors.)
In
some places, navigational traditions became inextricable from spiritual
cosmologies. The Europeans who settled Australia considered the
Aboriginal peoples to be idle wanderers of the bush, but in fact many of
them travelled along songlines—paths
with songs attached to them that commemorate the passage of primordial
beings who created the world. The words of the songs described the
continent and the routes across it. One Aboriginal group, in
particular—speakers of Guugu Yimithirr, a traditional language of Far
North Queensland—uses an absolute rather than an egocentric perspective
to describe space (in other words, not “Move to your left” but “Move
southeast”). According to the psycholinguist Stephen Levinson, this has
given them an almost superhuman capacity to orient themselves, night or
day, using both relatively commonplace cues, such as sun and seasonal
winds, and more specialized ones, such as the appearance of sand dunes
and termite mounds. Levinson concluded, with admiration, that the Guugu
Yimithirr speakers achieve “in software what pigeons apparently achieve
in hardware.”
Many of the world’s
navigation systems have been lost to time or replaced with
technology—or, in the case of the songlines, damaged through cultural
oppression. For the British author and self-styled “natural navigator”
Tristan Gooley, their disappearance signifies a cultural and
philosophical impoverishment. “By using a GPS to find our way instead of
clues available in the world itself, we devalue the experience of
traveling anywhere,” he told me in an e-mail. And there may be
neurological consequences, too. We build cognitive maps in the
hippocampus, the same area in which episodic memory and future planning
take place. Advanced technologies insure that we use our brains as
little as possible. In a series of studies in 2010, a group of
researchers at McGill University, in Montreal, reported that exercising
spatial memory and way-finding in everyday life increases hippocampal
function and gray matter, whereas underuse of these functions in older
adults may contribute to cognitive impairment. (One of the researchers,
Véronique Bohbot, told the Boston Globe that she no longer uses satellite-navigation devices.)
As
part of his course, Huth asks his students to study the night sky. This
spring, they learned the coördinates of some twenty-two stars and their
celestial paths, then went to the roof of the Harvard University
Science Center to identify a handful of them. What he has found over the
course of eight years of teaching primitive navigation, Huth told me,
is that the more attuned to the environment his students become, the
more their awareness seems to expand. “Sometimes they’re engaging in
this material and experiencing an epiphany to other aspects of their
life,” he said. Louis Baum, a Ph.D. candidate in physics and a teaching
fellow for the course, told me that he and his colleagues find the same.
“We get philosophical about it—about how knowing where you are helps
you know your place in the world,” he said. Whereas the modern stargazer
is liable to look up with a sense of existential wonder, if not dread,
our ancestors may have seen in that lovely firmament a map of home.
On
the roof of the Science Center, Huth named the stars as they flickered
into view: Spica, Antares, Altair, Dubhe, Pollux. As he did so, a
student approached, brimming with excitement. He had recognized several
stars and measured their altitude and azimuth. “Before this, I was
looking at the stars online,” he said. “It’s actually a little easier
when you are up here and see it in real life.”
No comments:
Post a Comment